Monday, May 31, 2010

Fear: 2. Individual response patterns

Fear is the emotional response to person, place or thing perceived as threatening the integrity of the organism.  Fear is the  “early warning system” alerting the organism to imminent danger. Feelings of personal vulnerability switch on instinctive response patterns.

No contagious disease spreads as fast as fear does into a group. As seen amongst many animal groups in the wild, the high arousal of fear results in flight en mass. Migratory herds used to travelling long distances in search of water and grazing pastures generally stampede at the first hint of danger from predators, depending on their speed and agility to outpace or evade the threat. Responses adopted over generations of existence are activated, as they literally run for their lives. When cornered however, they can turn on the aggression, using horns and hooves to fight their way out or protect their young.

Predators are not immune to fear, but respond differently to its stimulus, generally with focused aggression.  Their running capacities are more limited; they also are incapable of taking on the thundering hooves of stampeding herds. Hence they strategize, deliberately spooking the herds into flight so that the very young, the very old or disabled that invariably fall behind are separated from the rest, and attacked.


Fear thus generally stimulates two types of reactive behaviour – flight or fight. Human beings have also inherited these mechanisms of self-preservation. The emotional mind is tuned to understanding whole body movements, gestures and facial expressions. Time is not lost in questioning motives or assessing alternatives, as individuals or in groups. As in other animals, the responses made are instinctive, animalistic behaviour inherited over ages of species learning. There is an unquestioned following of what others are doing, whether it is running away with the crowd or indulging in mob violence.

Fear often influences our choices, activating the unthinking routes of reactive behaviour whereby unprocessed ‘gut feelings’ are acted upon. The instinctive behaviour can, however, be overridden by conscious critical thought. The biological difference that put human beings at the top of the evolutionary tree is the mental capacity of information processing. Emotions and feelings generated unconsciously may be examined, analyzed and evaluated against the present circumstances. The awareness of duty and training, for example, enables fire fighters to rush into burning buildings despite the obvious dangers to the self.

Organized societies have drastically changed the surrounding environments. Human beings are no longer at the mercy of natural forces and predators of the wild as in the remote past. With proliferations of building structures causing natural forests to shrink, survival now is predominantly psychological in the city concrete jungles. Threats are perceived in terms of interpersonal relationships, financial stability and target achievements at work.

Technological advancements have far outpaced human evolution. Unpredictable global change tests their adaptability. Stress is on the rise, with information overload, time pressures and ultimately, poor decision-making. Fear occupies the mind – fear of spaces, fear of public speaking, fear of failure, fear of intimacy, fear of losing out, so on and so forth to fear of fear itself. Eventually, the paralyzing effects of unprocessed fear leads to unthinking behaviour, reactive, instinctive and animalistic. 

Fear in extreme becomes a phobia, whereby the individual is reduced to a blue funk at sight or sound of the stressor. This conditioned response was seen in the classic experiments with “Little Albert”, an infant about eight months old:

Watson wanted to determine if a loud sound would cause a fear response in the child.  He was placed in a room and an experimenter stood behind him and made a loud noise by striking a hammer on a steel bar.  The first time this was done, Albert startled and raised his hands up.  The second time, he began to tremble, and on the third time he was crying and having a fit. 

A couple of months later, psychologist JB Watson experimented with conditioning an emotional response in the child:

He was first presented with a white rat.  When he reached out to touch it, the bar was struck.  The child fell forward, but did not cry.  He reached for the animal again, and the noise was made a second time.  This time little Albert cried.   One week later, he was presented with the rat again.  This time he did not reach for it immediately.  Instead, the rat was placed closer to him.  Then he slowly reached for it, but snatched his hand away before making contact with it.  The rat was presented again and Albert cried at the sight of the rat alone.

Eventually the child’s fearful response extended to all things furry!

Fear is a survival tool that is meant to alert the organism to novel or unexpected influences. However, with emotional conditioning even normally reasonable individuals may become overwhelmed, and triggered by a stressor, behave like deer caught in the headlights. The initial unpleasant experience that caused the conditioning may be forgotten, yet the irrational behavioural responses persist.

To deal with the fear, the source of the sense of vulnerability needs to be realized to enable the person to desensitize to its implications. Unprocessed fears push the making of instinctive choices that tend to be regretted later. The use of reason enables the individual to put feelings and emotions in perspective, and appropriately utilize the energies generated. Else, in the words of Louis E Boone:

The saddest summary of life contains 3 descriptions – could have, might have, should have…



References for this post:
  1. Sengupta Biswas, Jharna “Group Contagiontwmacademy.com TWM Academy. Powered by The Working Manager Ltd. 
  2. Sense of Feartwmacademy.com The Diva column. TWM Academy. Powered by The Working Manager Ltd. 
  3. Little Albertsbc.
Next...organizational

Monday, May 10, 2010

Fear: 1. ‘Alert’ and ‘ paralyse’

Albeit fanciful, the success of horror films centres on generating terror in viewers. Cine-goers appreciate the vicarious thrill of being in dangerous situations, being terrorized and helpless to resist. The makers of “Phoonk2” a macabre film that released in India recently, claimed nobody could sit through a screening alone and unafraid. A reward was thus offered to anybody able to challenge the claim.

The catch: throughout the
viewing at a large-screen theatre, the contestants would be hooked up to EEG machines to monitor changes of their heart rate. The differences in the readings obtained before and after viewing the film would detect fear being generated by the movie content. They would, in other words, have to remain entirely unmoved.  As it happened, those who signed up to prove their valour couldn’t control their heart rates and hence, the reward money remained unclaimed! 



I am reminded of the reliability of lie detector tests. The test administrators pick up changes from resting conditions as signs of subjective guilt. The tests however, are not infallible because they might detect signs of stress with the questioning rather than spot the specific lies they are supposed to. The same must be said about the filmmaker’s test; what it measures is not necessarily abject fear.

Heart rate is just one among the many signs. Besides all cardiac changes are not attributable to that fear syndrome, they could be from other conditions, especially stress of some kind. In normal individuals, the initial change from the resting condition is due to a general arousal in the system. The individual becomes alert; their faculties sharpened to focus on the incoming stimuli. In the theatre showing the horror movie, surround-sound acoustics and larger-than-life visuals would bombard the viewer’s senses – eyes and ears in particular. The physiological change recorded would then be an autonomic response alerting the system to the dramatic sensory input.

Certainly, people caught in threatening situations feel fear that raises their heart rates, along with other noticeable physiological responses like clammy hands, dry mouth, hoarse throat, facial expressions, staring eyes, and so on. Abject fear also causes emotional paralysis and inability to move, like a rabbit on the highway. The several signs measured together would have proved the point conclusively and should indeed have been included in the test.

In every day vocabulary, the word ‘fear’ is connoted to mean something bad, dangerous and threatening. Similarly, ‘stress’, readily associated with negativity and debilitating conditions, invokes images of things that catch people by the throat and need to be avoided. Fact is the words ‘stress’ and ‘fear’, are simply denotative, and can have both positive and negative implications. 

For example, the word ‘stress’ also means ‘to emphasize’ which can be good. To initiate awareness, any incoming sensory stimulus creates a degree of stress. The organism is alerted, to focus attention on new influences. This is eustress, good stress, or to distinguish from the other, “fear 2”. The concomitant physiological changes serve to collect and process data, and eventually to motivate action. When the incoming stimulus is positive, the degree of arousal stays low, serving to sharpen the performance faculties.

Molly Gordon explains the value of fear 2:

  • Clarity and purpose
  • Stepping out of comfort zone
  • Capacity to respond to danger
  • Moving forward powerfully

The alerting reaction enables adaptability. Normally, the physical senses: sight, hearing, touch, smell and taste, are engaged in monitoring the environment. Any novel stimulus, like an unusual sound heard, or flash seen triggers a general ‘alarm’ or systemic arousal. Immediately other physical and mental resources pitch to locate the source of the stimulus, and determine its importance, implications for individual wellbeing and possible courses of action. 

The choice and motivation to deal with the stimulus is entirely personal. In Zen meditation, for example, the exponent becomes one with the universe. Although every external change evokes an internal awareness, the reaction to it may be controlled. In Yogic meditation, the exponent withdraws attention altogether from the external surroundings, and remains unreceptive and unresponsive to the incoming stimuli.

A stimulus is negative when the possible changes in status quo it will cause are perceived harmful. The system gears up to combat the inputs and resist its influence. If it should succeed, the threat has been confronted and dealt with, and equilibrium returns. When the individual ability to cope is in doubt, the degree of arousal becomes excessive leading to distress, bad stress that can be recorded on several physiological channels, or as some postulate the difference, “fear 1”.

Gordon outlines some negative effects of fear 1:

  • Panic and confusion
  • Inability to move
  • Magnified danger and vulnerability
  • Emotional outpourings
The bad stress reaction feeds on itself to become unmanageable. The organism proceeds to exhaustion and displays obvious signs of losing equilibrium. To a large extent, the distress is psychological. Neurobiologist Joseph LeDoux points out that the emotional mind plays a large role in the response to fear. The pathway of neural processing is different in eustress and distress. In the latter, the cascading effect of the stimulus-response loop turns off the higher brain centres. The individual is unable to think clearly, becomes increasingly stereotypical and ineffective in action.



These are the clear signs of terror the genre of scary movies ‘kill’ for. To prove the point about his horror movie, the filmmaker needed to include the other physiological parameters in the test. But perhaps that was not quite the intent. The blanket attribution made to “fear” in every change of heart rate perceived in the test was useful to save the prize money. It also advertised the film, and claimed for it a rating higher than that its content might actually deserve.


References for this post:

  1. Gordon Molly “Dealing With Fear and Anxiety: Principle 3 - Discern Two Types of Fearmollygordon.com HOW TO OVERCOME FEARS AND ANXIETY. Shaboom Inc. 2003-2006 
  2. Sense of Fear” twmacademy.com The Diva Column. TWM Academy. Powered by The Working Manager Ltd. 
  3. Sengupta Biswas, Jharna “Stress in modern timestwmacademy.com TWM Academy. Powered by The Working Manager Ltd. 
  4. Types of stress – Some Good Some Badstressfocus.com Stressfocus 2010. 
Next…response patterns